2024-09-04 Wed
The effect of television violence on boys’ aggression was investigated with consideration of teacher-rated characteristic aggressiveness, timing of frustration, and violence-related cues as moderators. Boys in Grades 2 and 3 (N=396) watched violent or nonviolent TV in groups of 6, and half the groups were later exposed to a cue associated with the violent TV program…
…They were frustrated either before or after TV viewing. Aggression was measured by naturalistic observation during a game of floor hockey. Groups containing more characteristically high-aggressive boys showed higher aggression following violent TV plus the cue than following violent TV alone, which in turn produced more aggression than did the nonviolent TV condition…
…There was evidence that both the violent content and the cue may have suppressed aggression among groups composed primarily of boys low in characteristic aggressiveness. Results were interpreted in terms of current information-processing theories of media effects on aggression.
\(b_1*charAggr + b_2*vioTV +... = aggrObs + err\)
…Groups containing more characteristically high-aggressive boys showed higher aggression following violent TV plus the cue than following violent TV alone…
\[aggrChar + vioTV + cue >\]
\[aggrChar + vioTV + noCue\]
How science works (or should)
…and it has to be shown why that other theory can or cannot be the true one: And until this is shown, and until we know how it is shown, we do not understand the grounds of our opinion.
John Stuart Mill quoted in Ritchie (2020), p. 14
…Only when certain events recur in accordance with rules or regularities, as is the case with repeatable experiments, can our observations be tested–in principle–by anyone. We do not take even our own observations quite seriously, or accept them as scientific observations, until we have repeated and test them…
Karl Popper quoted in Ritchie (2020), p. 23
…Only by such repetitions can we convince ourselves that we are not dealing with a mere isolated ‘coincidence’…
Karl Popper quoted in Ritchie (2020), p. 23
Science’s social nature does come with weaknesses, however. Because scientists focus so much on trying to persuade their peers, which is the way they get those studies through peer review and oward to publication, it’s all too easy for them to disregard the real object of science: getting us closer to the truth.
Ritchie (2020), Chapter 1, pp. 14-15
Existing norms for scientific communication are rooted in anachronistic practices of bygone eras making them needlessly inefficient. We outline a path that moves away from the existing model of scientific communication to improve the efficiency in meeting the purpose of public science—knowledge accumulation.
Nosek & Bar-Anan (2012)
We call for six changes: (a) full embrace of digital communication; (b) open access to all published research; (c) disentangling publication from evaluation; (d) breaking the “one article, one journal” model with a grading system for evaluation and diversified dissemination outlets; (e) publishing peer review; and (f) allowing open, continuous peer review.
Nosek & Bar-Anan (2012)
We address conceptual and practical barriers to change and provide examples showing how the suggested practices are being used already. The critical barriers to change are not technical or financial; they are social. Although scientists guard the status quo, they also have the power to change it.
Nosek & Bar-Anan (2012)
Nosek & Bar-Anan (2012), , pp. 25-26
Work Session: Reading a paper; Evaluating its claims
Watching the talk by Nosek is not required. But he’s a very good speaker and an inspiring person.
This talk was produced using Quarto, using the RStudio Integrated Development Environment (IDE) version 2024.4.2.764.
The source files are in R and R Markdown, then rendered to HTML using the revealJS framework.