Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
71(2), 230–244.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.71.2.230
Begley, C. G. (2013). Six red flags for suspect work.
Nature,
497(7450), 433–434.
https://doi.org/10.1038/497433a
Begley, C. G., & Ellis, L. M. (2012). Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research.
Nature,
483(7391), 531–533.
https://doi.org/10.1038/483531a
Brainerd, J., & You, J. (2018). What a massive database of retracted papers reveals about science publishing’s
“death penalty.” Science.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav8384
Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., … Wu, H. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in
Nature and
Science between 2010 and 2015.
Nature Human Behaviour, 1.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z
Carey, M. A., Steiner, K. L., & Petri, W. A., Jr. (2020). Ten simple rules for reading a scientific paper.
PLoS Computational Biology,
16(7), e1008032.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008032
Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Yap, A. J. (2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance.
Psychological Science,
21(10), 1363–1368.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610383437
Carpenter, S. (2012). Harvard psychology researcher committed fraud,
US investigation concludes.
Science,
6. Retrieved from
https://www.science.org/content/article/harvard-psychology-researcher-committed-fraud-us-investigation-concludes
Claesen, A., Gomes, S., Tuerlinckx, F., & Vanpaemel, W. (2021). Comparing dream to reality: An assessment of adherence of the first generation of preregistered studies.
Royal Society Open Science,
8(211037).
https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.211037
Collaboration, O. S. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science.
Science,
349(6251), aac4716.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716
Crüwell, S., Doorn, J. van, Etz, A., Makel, M. C., Moshontz, H., Niebaum, J. C., … Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M. (2019). Seven easy steps to open science.
Zeitschrift für Psychologie,
227(4), 237–248.
https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000387
Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C.-L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind, but whose mind?
PloS One,
7(1), e29081.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029081
Earp, B. D., Everett, J. A. C., Madva, E. N., & Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Out, damned spot: Can the
“Macbeth effect” be replicated?
Basic and Applied Social Psychology,
36(1), 91–98.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2013.856792
Errington, T. M., Denis, A., Perfito, N., Iorns, E., & Nosek, B. A. (2021). Challenges for assessing replicability in preclinical cancer biology.
eLife,
10, e67995.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67995
Errington, T. M., Mathur, M., Soderberg, C. K., Denis, A., Perfito, N., Iorns, E., & Nosek, B. A. (2021). Investigating the replicability of preclinical cancer biology.
eLife,
10, e71601.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.71601
Fidler, F., & Wilcox, J. (2021). Reproducibility of scientific results. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.),
The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2021). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. Retrieved from
https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2021/entries/scientific-reproducibility/
FORRT - framework for open and reproducible research training. (n.d.).
https://forrt.org/. Retrieved from
https://forrt.org/
Franco, A., Malhotra, N., & Simonovits, G. (2014). Social science. Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer.
Science,
345(6203), 1502–1505.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1255484
Gilmore, R. O., & Adolph, K. E. (2017). Video can make behavioural research more reproducible.
Nature Human Behavior,
1.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0128
Gilmore, R. O., Cole, P. M., Verma, S., Aken, M. A. G., & Worthman, C. M. (2020). Advancing scientific integrity, transparency, and openness in child development research: Challenges and possible solutions.
Child Development Perspectives,
14(1), 9–14.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12360
Gilroy, S. P., & Kaplan, B. A. (2019). Furthering open science in behavior analysis: An introduction and tutorial for using
GitHub in research.
Perspectives on Behavior Science,
42(3), 565–581.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-019-00202-5
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2016). Why preregistration makes me nervous.
APS Observer,
29(7). Retrieved from
https://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/why-preregistration-makes-me-nervous
Goodman, S. N., Fanelli, D., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2016). What does research reproducibility mean?
Science Translational Medicine,
8(341), 341ps12–341ps12.
https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.aaf5027
Houtkoop, B. L., Chambers, C., Macleod, M., Bishop, D. V. M., Nichols, T. E., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2018). Data sharing in psychology: A survey on barriers and preconditions.
Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2515245917751886.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245917751886
John, L. K., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2012). Measuring the prevalence of questionable research practices with incentives for truth telling.
Psychological Science,
23(5), 524–532.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611430953
Kardash, C. M., & Edwards, O. V. (2012). Thinking and behaving like scientists: Perceptions of undergraduate science interns and their faculty mentors.
Instructional Science,
40(6), 875–899.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-011-9195-0
Kathawalla, U.-K., Silverstein, P., & Syed, M. (2021). Easing into open science: A guide for graduate students and their advisors.
Collabra. Psychology,
7(1).
https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.18684
Ledgerwood, A. (2018). The preregistration revolution needs to distinguish between predictions and analyses.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
115(45), E10516–E10517.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1812592115
Levelt, W. J. M., Drenth, P. J. D., & Noort, E. (2012). Flawed science: The fraudulent research practices of social psychologist diederik stapel.
https://pure.mpg.de/rest/items/item_1569964/component/file_1569966/content; pure.mpg.de. Retrieved from
https://pure.mpg.de/rest/items/item_1569964/component/file_1569966/content
Macfarlane, B., & Cheng, M. (2008). Communism, universalism and disinterestedness: Re-examining contemporary support among academics for merton’s scientific norms.
Journal of Academic Ethics,
6(1), 67–78.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10805-008-9055-y
Merton, R. W. (1973). The normative structure of science. In R. K. Merton & N. W. Storer (Eds.), The Sociology of Science: Theoretical and Empirical Investigations (pp. 267–278). The University of Chicago Press.
Meyer, M. N. (2018). Practical tips for ethical data sharing.
Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2515245917747656.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245917747656
Mitroff, I. I. (1974). Norms and counter-norms in a select group of the
Apollo moon scientists: A case study of the ambivalence of scientists.
American Sociological Review,
39(4), 579–595.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2094423
Munafò, M. R., Nosek, B. A., Bishop, D. V. M., Button, K. S., Chambers, C. D., Sert, N. P. du, … Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). A manifesto for reproducible science.
Nature Human Behaviour,
1, 0021.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0021
Ngiam, W. (2020, April).
ReproducibiliTea | simmons, nelson and simonsohn (2011).
False-Positive psychology. Youtube. Retrieved from
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=bf3GqyBRgzY
Nosek, B. A., Alter, G., Banks, G. C., Borsboom, D., Bowman, S. D., Breckler, S. J., … Yarkoni, T. (2015). Promoting an open research culture.
Science,
348(6242), 1422–1425.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aab2374
Nosek, Brian A., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2012). Scientific utopia i: Opening scientific communication.
Psychological Inquiry,
23(3), 217–243.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.692215
Nosek, Brian A., Ebersole, C. R., DeHaven, A. C., & Mellor, D. T. (2018). The preregistration revolution.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
115(11), 2600–2606.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1708274114
Nosek, Brian A., Hardwicke, T. E., Moshontz, H., Allard, A., Corker, K. S., Dreber, A., … Vazire, S. (2022). Replicability, robustness, and reproducibility in psychological science.
Annual Review of Psychology,
73(2022), 719–748.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-114157
Nuijten, M. B., Hartgerink, C. H. J., Assen, M. A. L. M. van, Epskamp, S., & Wicherts, J. M. (2015). The prevalence of statistical reporting errors in psychology (1985–2013).
Behavior Research Methods, 1–22.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0664-2
Oreskes, N. (2019). Why Trust Science. Princeton University Press.
Ranehill, E., Dreber, A., Johannesson, M., Leiberg, S., Sul, S., & Weber, R. A. (2015). Assessing the robustness of power posing: No effect on hormones and risk tolerance in a large sample of men and women.
Psychological Science,
26(5), 653–656.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614553946
Ritchie, S. (2020).
Science fictions: Exposing fraud, bias, negligence and hype in science (1st ed.). Penguin Random House. Retrieved from
https://www.amazon.com/Science-Fictions/dp/1847925669
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results.
Psychological Bulletin,
86(3), 638–641.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638
Ruben, A. (2016). How to read a scientific paper.
Science| AAAS [Internet],
20. Retrieved from
https://www.science.org/content/article/how-read-scientific-paper-rev2
Sagan, C. (1996). The Demon-haunted World: Science as a Candle in the Dark (pp. 200–218). Ballantine Books.
Silberzahn, R., Uhlmann, E. L., Martin, D. P., Anselmi, P., Aust, F., Awtrey, E., … Nosek, B. A. (2018). Many analysts, one data set: Making transparent how variations in analytic choices affect results.
Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science,
1(3), 337–356.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245917747646
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology:
Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant.
Psychological Science,
22(11), 1359–1366.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417632
Soska, K. C., Xu, M., Gonzalez, S. L., Herzberg, O., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Gilmore, R. O., & Adolph, K. E. (2021). (Hyper)active data curation: A video case study from behavioral science.
Journal of Escience Librarianship,
10(3).
https://doi.org/10.7191/jeslib.2021.1208
Szucs, D., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature.
PLoS Biology,
15(3), e2000797.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2000797
Tenopir, C., Rice, N. M., Allard, S., Baird, L., Borycz, J., Christian, L., … Sandusky, R. J. (2020). Data sharing, management, use, and reuse: Practices and perceptions of scientists worldwide.
PloS One,
15(3), e0229003.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229003
Zhong, C.-B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins:
Threatened morality and physical cleansing.
Science,
313(5792), 1451–1452.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130726